Get 20M+ Full-Text Papers For Less Than $1.50/day. Start a 14-Day Trial for You or Your Team.

Learn More →

Adolescent gambling behaviour, a single latent construct and indicators of risk: findings from a national survey of New Zealand high school students

Adolescent gambling behaviour, a single latent construct and indicators of risk: findings from a... f.rossen@auckland.ac.nz Social and Community This study explores underlying latent construct/s of gambling behaviour, and identifies Health, School of Population indicators of “unhealthy gambling”. Data were collected from Youth’07 a nationally rep- Health, Faculty of Medical and Health Sciences, resentative sample of New Zealand secondary school students (N = 9107). Exploratory University of Auckland, factor analyses, item-response theory analyses, multiple indicators-multiple causes, and Private Bag 92019, differential item functioning analyses were used to assess dimensionality of gambling Auckland 1142, New Zealand Full list of author information behaviour, underlying factors, and indicators of unhealthy gambling. A single underly- is available at the end of the ing continuum of gambling behaviour was identified. Gambling frequency and ‘gam- article bling because I can’t stop’ were most strongly associated with unhealthy gambling. Gambling to ‘feel better about myself ’ and to ‘forget about things’ provided the most precise discriminants of unhealthy gambling. Multivariable analyses found that school connectedness was associated with lower levels of unhealthy gambling. Background Gambling has become a widely available activity in modern societies (Turchi and Der- evensky 2006). Evidence suggests that it has become a popular past-time, not only for adults, but also for children and adolescents (Derevensky and Gupta 2000; Gupta and Derevensky 1998a; Jacobs 2000; Splevins et  al. 2010; Turchi and Derevensky 2006). Whilst for many youth involvement in gambling does not result in problematic behav- iour, other adolescents go on to experience serious issues as a result of their gambling. While the documented rates of youth involvement in gambling vary widely (perhaps due to methodological discrepancies), a recent review of studies of young people’s gambling in North America, Europe and Oceania reported that 20–90  % of young people had gambled in the past year (Volberg et al. 2010) and 0.8–13.0 % were involved in problem/ pathological gambling (Volberg et al. 2010). Certain groups of young people are at greater risk of unhealthy gambling. For instance, male youth are more likely than their female counterparts to engage in gambling (Blinn- Pike et  al. 2010; Darling et  al. 2006; Delfabbro et  al. 2005; Huang and Boyer 2007; Raisamo et  al. 2013; Turchi and Derevensky 2006), gamble more frequently (Delfabbro et  al. 2005; Gupta and Derevensky 1998a, b) and do so on a wider range of activities © 2016 The Author(s). This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 2 of 12 (Delfabbro et  al. 2005). The prevalence of problem gambling amongst youth from dif - ferent ethnic groups has not been widely investigated (Blinn-Pike et  al. 2010). Despite this lack of evidence, a few studies have indicated that indigenous youth and those from non-majority ethnic groups may be less likely to gamble than their peers. However those non-majority ethnic youth that do gamble are at greater risk of experiencing problem gambling (Delfabbro et al. 2005; Volberg et al. 2010). The literature has highlighted that adolescent problem gamblers have an array of co- existing problems (Gupta and Derevensky 2000; Shead et al. 2010; Volberg et al. 2010), with researchers theorising that gambling is often undertaken as an attempt to manage or resolve other underlying issues (Gupta and Derevensky 2000; Ste-Marie et al. 2002). Furthermore, risk factors associated with problem gambling, such as mental health issues (e.g. depression and anxiety) may also be observed in other potentially harmful behaviours in adolescence, for instance substance misuse (Derevensky et al. 2003; Shead et  al. 2010). While there has been some focus on the identification of risk factors for youth problem gambling, little is known about the protective factors that insulate young people against unhealthy gambling (Dickson et al. 2008), or the extent of their positive influence over risk factors. There is a dearth of information on the ‘warning signs’ for problem gambling, mean - ing that preventive measures can be hampered. Policy makers and those working clini- cally in the problem gambling field would benefit from an exploration of indicators of when gambling behaviour is becoming unhealthy, and a non-dichotomised examination of youth gambling would allow for a comprehensive analysis of factors associated with unhealthy gambling. This study presents findings from research which explored youth participation in gambling and the impact of problem gambling, and identifiable risk and resiliency factors. To the best of the authors’ knowledge this is the first systematic assess - ment of risk and protective factors of youth gambling based on data from a nationally representative sample of young people. The objectives of this study were to: (1) explore the existence of an underlying latent construct/s of gambling behaviour—i.e. to create an underlying latent continuum of gambling behaviours from ‘less unhealthy’ to ‘more unhealthy’; (2) identify ‘red flags’, or indicators of unhealthy gambling behaviours; and, (3) explore and identify associations between gambling behaviour (as measured by the latent construct), demographic variables, and variables that were hypothesised to fulfil risk or protective factors. Methods Sample Data for this study were collected as part of Youth’07, a nationally representative sur- vey of the health and wellbeing of New Zealand secondary school students conducted in 2007. A brief overview of the Youth’07 survey design and methodology is included below, while a comprehensive description has been provided elsewhere (Adolescent Health Research Group 2008). A total of 9107 randomly selected secondary schools students (equating to 3.4  % of the total 2007 New Zealand secondary school roll) aged between 12 and 19 years old completed the survey. Final response rates were 84 % for schools and 74 % for students. The comprehensive 622-question survey was administered via inter - net tablets (hand held computers). Screening questions were used to ensure that only Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 3 of 12 those who had experienced a particular issue were asked more detailed questions (e.g. those who had not smoked cigarettes were not asked further questions about smoking). Hence, students generally answered considerably fewer questions than the survey total number of questions. Measures Involvement in gambling was assessed through a series of questions. The first question determined past year gambling: (“In the last year have you ever gambled or bet money on things like Lotto, Instant Kiwi, Pokies etc., or bet money with friends?”). Students who answered affirmatively were then asked five questions to determine the extent of their involvement in gambling behaviour over the past year, in particular: • Gambling activities “In the last year have you gambled or bet money on any of these?” Students could select as many of the following options as appropriate—Instant Kiwi (scratchies); Lotto (including Strike and Powerball); bingo or housie; pub or club pok- ies (i.e. electronic gambling machines/EGMs); casino pokies or tables (e.g. roulette); TAB betting; internet (e.g. internet casinos); bets with friends; 0900 phone games; cards or coins; other; and/or, none of these. • Frequency of gambling “During the last four weeks, about how often did you gam- ble?” Affirmative responses to this item were aggregated to ‘several times a week/ most days a week,’ and the broader category ‘once a week or less’. • Money spent on gambling “How much money would you usually spend each week on bets or gambling?” Responses to this item were collapsed into two categories—‘less than NZ$20’ (less than approximately US$13) and ‘NZ$20 or more’. • Time spent gambling “How much time would you usually spend each day on bets or gambling?” Responses were aggregated to ‘less than 1 h a day’ and ‘1 or more hours per day’. • Reasons for gambling “Why do you gamble or bet money?” Students were able to select as many reasons as appropriate from the following list: “to have fun”; “to win money”; “because I am bored”; “to relax”; “to feel better about myself ”; “to forget about things”; “because my friends do”; “because my family does”; “for a challenge”; “because I can’t stop”; “because I am short of money”; “to get a buzz”; and, “none of these”. Variables hypothesised to fulfil risk or protective factors Variables selected for inclusion in this set of analyses were identified a priori by review - ing the literature and based on consultation with the study’s advisory group, which con- sisted of researchers and clinicians with expertise in the prevention and treatment of problem gambling. • Wellbeing was assessed by the WHO-Five Wellbeing Index (Bech et  al. 2003). This Index is made up of five items that use a six-point Likert scale, from 0 (at no time) to 6 (all of the time). Three underlying constructs are measured: positive mood, vital - ity, and general interests. Response scores were summed to provide an overall score, with higher scores indicating greater wellbeing. Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 4 of 12 • Depression was measured by the Reynolds Adolescent Depression Scale-Short Form (RADS-SF) (Norris 2007). This scale consists of ten items, each of which has four response options: “almost never” “hardly ever”, “sometimes”, and “most of the time” (significant depressive symptoms  ≥ 28, total score = 40). The overall score provides an indication of affective status, with higher scores being an indicator of greater depressive symptomatology. • Data on weekly alcohol use were gathered by asking those students who had drank alcohol at least once in their lifetime “During the past 4 weeks, about how often did you drink alcohol?”; responses to this item were aggregated to ‘weekly or more often’ and ‘less than weekly’. • Scales were developed to provide measures of social connectedness in three domains: family, friends, and school. The scale for family connectedness included nine items that focused on: whether students had fun with their family; if they got along with their family; and, their relationship with their family, in particular their mother and/ or father (or people that functioned as parents). The scale for connectedness to friends incorporated six items which covered various aspects of friendships, such as having caring friends and fun with friends. The school connectedness scale included eight items that enquired about students’ connectedness to their school. All three scales demonstrated adequate to good internal consistency, with Cronbach’s alphas of 0.85 (family), 0.78 (friends) and 0.60 (school). • Weekly income there was no specific question about whether or not students received a weekly allowance, hence income was determined by asking students “How much money do you usually earn each week?” • Age, sex, and ethnicity were self-reported. • Neighbourhood socio-economic deprivation was measured using the New Zealand Deprivation Index (NZDep2006) (Salmond et al. 2006). NZDep2006 was also used to identify whether students resided in urban or rural settings. Analysis The analyses for this study were completed in 2015 and includes only those students who had gambled in the past year from the total Youth’07 sample of 9107 students (26.7  %; n  =  2234). These students were significantly (p  ≤  0.05) more likely to be male (31.3  % male and 21.5 % female), older (24.9 % ‘13 or less’, 23.7 % ‘14 years old’, 26.2 % ‘15 years old’, 28.5 % ‘16 years old’, 31.9 % ‘17 or older’), and from neighbourhoods with lower lev- els of socio-economic deprivation (28.2 % low, 27.5 % medium, 23.3 % high). No differ - ences were observed for past-year gambling status according to ethnicity or rural/urban category. Exploratory factor analysis was used to assess dimensionality of gambling behaviour and the number of underlying factors. The relationship between participants’ responses to each item and their level on the underlying latent gambling continuum/s were exam- ined using two-parameter logistic item-response theory (IRT) models. Item characteris- tic curves (ICC) illustrate these relationships, which were characterised by item severity and discrimination parameters. Item severity provides an indication of the position of the item characteristic curve in relation to the latent continuum: higher severity val- ues indicate that an item is associated with a higher severity of gambling behaviour (i.e. Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 5 of 12 unhealthy gambling behaviour). Discrimination parameters provide an indication of the accuracy or precision of each item in distinguishing between those participants with lev- els of the latent continuum above, and those participants with levels below, the item’s severity. A low discrimination estimate indicates that an item is unrelated to the under- lying construct or that the item is poorly defined. Multiple indicators-multiple causes (MIMIC) analyses were used to examine the rela- tionship between demographic variables and the underlying continuum of problem gambling behaviours and predictors of unhealthy gambling behaviour. Differential Item Functioning (DIF) by demographic variables (i.e. age, sex and ethnicity) was used to examine differences in how demographic groups respond to individual questions, inde - pendent of the underlying problem gambling continuum. The presence of DIF would indicate measurement non-equivalence or item response bias across groups. Analysis of risk and protective factors progressed in two models. The first model examined independent associations between the latent gambling continuum and risk and protective factors, whilst accounting for student-level demographics. In model two, the association of all significant risk and protective factors and student-level demo - graphics were examined simultaneously in relation to the latent gambling continuum. Multivariable, factor, IRT, and MIMIC analyses were undertaken using Mplus (Muthen and Muthen 1998–2007). All analyses accounted for the clustering of students within the same school and the unequal probability of selection. Human participant protection Principals provided consent on behalf of their schools and students that were selected and their parents were provided with information sheets about the study. Students provided their consent on the day of participating. Ethical approval for the study was granted by the University of Auckland Human Subject Ethics Committee. Results Underlying constructs of unhealthy gambling behaviour Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was used to assess the dimensionality of the gambling behaviour items relating to reasons for gambling, frequency of gambling, and time and money spent gambling. The scree plot of the eigenvalues (5.301, 0.838, 0.294) and the ratio of the first to the second eigenvalue (5.301/0.838  = 6.326) suggested the presence of a single, dominant factor underlying these gambling items. This one-factor model demonstrated excellent fit indices (RMSEA  = 0.047, CFI = 0.982, TLI = 0.973), and pro- vided evidence of undimensionality of the variables, meaning that it was suitable for IRT modelling (see Table  1 for details on item factor loadings, discrimination and severity parameters). This factor was conceptualised as an underlying continuum of ‘unhealthy’ gambling behaviour and centres on motivations/reasons for gambling and levels of expenditure (in terms of time, frequency, and money) on gambling. Identifying the ‘red flags’ of unhealthy gambling behaviour Figure  1 illustrates the item characteristic curves for the gambling items. All of the gambling items exhibited good to high discriminative power (range 1.284–5.044) in Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 6 of 12 Table 1 Two-parameter item-response theory model of gambling behaviour Prevalence Factor loadings Item discrimination Item severity (%) Estimate Standard error Estimate Standard error I gamble to relax 6.0 0.848 1.603 0.133 1.841 0.068 I gamble to feel 2.9 0.981 5.044 1.913 1.928 0.060 better about myself I gamble to forget 3.7 0.914 2.246 0.256 1.954 0.073 about things I gamble because 1.6 0.885 1.902 0.295 2.431 0.116 I can’t stop Gamble ‘several 2.6 0.806 1.361 0.206 2.412 0.159 times a week’ or ‘most days’ Spend $20 or 5.0 0.789 1.284 0.123 2.101 0.100 more per week on gambling Spend 1 or more 3.1 0.866 1.730 0.240 2.189 0.101 hours per day gambling Fig. 1 Item response curve distinguishing behaviour along the gambling continuum, from less unhealthy to more unhealthy behaviour. Gambling “to feel better about myself ” was the item with the high- est discrimination (5.044, note the steepest slope in Fig.  1) and was most able to accu- rately discriminate students along the gambling continuum. Gambling “to forget about things”, “because I can’t stop”, and “to relax” also had high item discriminations (2.246, 1.902, and 1.603 respectively). The amount of money spent on gambling had the lowest item discrimination (1.284; note the least steep slope), meaning that it was least able to accurately discriminate students along the continuum of gambling behaviour. As gam- bling “to relax”, “to feel better about myself ”, and “to forget about things” had the lowest item severities, these items may therefore act as early indicators that gambling behaviour is more unhealthy. Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 7 of 12 Unhealthy gambling, depression and wellbeing Measures of depression and wellbeing were used to determine construct validation. The underlying gambling behaviour continuum was significantly correlated with standard - ised measures of depression (RADS-SF) and wellbeing (WHO-Five Wellbeing Index). ‘More unhealthy’ gambling behaviour was positively associated with depression and negatively associated with wellbeing, in that students with ‘more unhealthy’ gambling behaviour had significantly higher depression scores (r  =  0.162, p  <  0.001) and signifi - cantly lower wellbeing scores (r = −0.113, p < 0.001) than students who were gambling at ‘less unhealthy’ levels. Unhealthy gambling behaviour and different demographic groups MIMIC modelling was carried out to determine if there were significant direct effects between demographic variables (i.e. age, sex, and ethnicity) and the gambling items, and whether any of the demographic subgroups were at increased or decreased risk of unhealthy gambling behaviours  (see Fig.  2). Differential Item Functioning (DIF) dem - onstrated item equivalence for seven gambling items (gambling “to relax”, gambling “to feel better”, gambling “to forget”, gambling “because can’t stop”, frequency of gam- bling, money spent gambling, and time spent gambling) across the age, sex and ethnic groups of students. The MIMIC model provided a good fit for the data (RMSEA  = 0.030, CFI = 0.979, TLI = 0.973) and ‘unhealthy gambling’ behaviour was shown to vary across each of the investigated demographic variables. Higher levels of ‘unhealthy gambling’ behaviour were associated with being younger, male, and being an ethnic minority. Risk and protective factors for gambling behaviour Analyses for Model One resulted in a set of variables that were significantly associated with scores on the gambling continuum (see Table  2 for estimates and standard errors) while adjusting for age, sex, ethnicity and neighbourhood socio-economic deprivation. Ten variables were associated with increased risk/higher scores on the latent gambling continuum (i.e. ‘more unhealthy’ gambling behaviour). Three domains were negatively associated with scores on the gambling continuum, i.e. they were protective with regard to unhealthy gambling behaviour: connectedness to family, connectedness to friends, and connectedness to school. As levels of connectedness in each of these domains increased, the mean score on the gambling continuum decreased. Model Two entailed a multivariate analysis between the latent gambling continuum, demographic variables, and all of the variables identified in Model One as independently fulfilling risk/protective functions. The analysis in Model Two aimed to determine if those variables identified as fulfilling risk/protective functions (as identified in Model One) would continue to maintain these functions in the presence of each other. The only variable to maintain its protective status was school connectedness (p  =  0.018). Vari- ables that continued to be significantly associated with increased risk included being of a Pacific ethnicity (p  = 0.009), earning more than NZ$100 per week (p = 0.031), being male (p  =  0.002), gambling because friends gamble (p  <  0.001), and gambling because family gambles (p < 0.001). Gambling at a casino (on EGMs or tables) and gambling over the Internet were significantly associated with an increased risk of ‘unhealthy gambling’ Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 8 of 12 Gamble to relax YOUNGER STUDENTS Gamble to feel better about -0.093* (-0.067) myself MALE vs. FEMALE Gamble to forget about things 0.202* (0.417) UNHEALTHY MAORI vs. EUROPEAN GAMBLING Gamble because I can’t stop 0.182* (0.478) BEHAVIOUR Gamble ‘several times a PACIFIC vs. EUROPEAN week’ or ‘most days’ 0.237* (0.870) ASIAN vs. EUROPEAN Spend $20 or more per week 0.109* (0.336) on gambling Spend 1 or more hours per Regression coefficient (standard error); *P value <0.05 day gambling Fig. 2 MIMIC model of unhealthy gambling behaviour. Relates to students who have gambled in the past 12 months (N = 2234) behaviour (p = 0.006 and p = 0.001 respectively). Table 2 provides estimates and stand- ard errors for each of these items. Discussion Based on data from a nationally representative sample of secondary school students, the current study has identified a single construct of unhealthy gambling behaviour and a number of ‘red flags’, or indicators, of when gambling behaviour is at or is close to unhealthy levels. For instance, “gambling to relax”, “gambling to feel better about myself ”, and “gambling to forget about things” are probable early indicators that gambling behav- iour may be at unhealthy levels. It may be useful to incorporate these ‘red flags’ in health promotion and treatment initiatives so that young people and those responsible for their health and wellbeing (e.g. teachers, parents, and clinicians) have an awareness of the warning signs linked to unhealthy gambling behaviour. The statistical model and ‘red flags’ that were identified in this research resemble a number of aspects of Blaszczynski and Nower’s (2002) second pathway—emotionally vulnerable problem gamblers, from their theoretical Pathway Model of problem and pathological gambling conducted with adults. They propose that emotionally vulner - able gamblers are motivated to participate in gambling “by a desire to modulate affective states and/or meet specific psychological needs” (p. 492). Recent research in adolescent problem gambling has further supported Blaszczynski and Nower’s Pathway Model (Gupta et  al. 2013), and as in previous research the three ‘red flags’ of unhealthy gam - bling identified in the present study all appear to centre on reducing states of arousal. Moreover, in our study unhealthy levels of gambling were associated with depression, poorer wellbeing, and weekly use of alcohol. Blaszczynski and Nower (2002) also argue that the underlying psychological dysfunction present in ‘pathway two’ gamblers makes Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 9 of 12 Table 2 Multi-model correlations between  risk and  protective factors for  unhealthy gam- bling behaviour Item/variable associated with increased Model 1 Model 2 risk of unhealthy gambling Estimate (Standard error)* Estimate (Standard error)* Demographics Age (younger vs. older) – −0.092 (0.065) Sex (male vs. female) – 0.580 (0.187)* Māori (vs. European) – 0.227 (0.177) Pacific (vs. European) – 0.736 (0.282)* Asian (vs. European) – 0.535 (0.288) Living in neighbourhood with higher levels of dep- – 0.044 (0.032) rivation (vs. lower levels of deprivation) Risk factors Usually earns $100 or more per week 0.387 (0.111)* 0.322 (0.150)* Gamble because My friends gamble 1.436 (0.122)* 1.130 (0.231)* My family gamble 1.53 (0.13)* 1.161 (0.237)* Have gambled on Bingo 1.102 (0.133)* 0.402 (0.296) Pub/club EGMs 0.972 (0.118)* 0.199 (0.269) Casino (EGMs or tables) 1.514 (0.158)* 0.939 (0.339)* Internet 1.32 (0.114)* 0.864 (0.256)* Phone 1.637 (0.18)* 0.548 (0.474) Satisfied depression criteria 0.496 (0.128)* 0.261 (0.266) Drinks alcohol on a weekly basis 0.649 (0.078)* 0.246 (0.175) Protective factors Connectedness to family −0.245 (0.052)* 0.068 (0.103) Connectedness to friends −0.33 (0.038)* −0.053 (0.103) Connectedness to school −0.412 (0.077)* −0.326 (0.137)* * Item/variable significant at p ≤ 0.01 Adjusted for age, sex, ethnicity and neighbourhood socio-economic deprivation (NZDep2006) them more resistant to treatment and that underlying vulnerabilities, such as substance misuse, must be addressed alongside gambling issues. As in the current study, prior work has indicated that those from non-majority ethnic groups (e.g. Native American and African American youth in the USA and indigenous young people in Australia) may not be any more likely to gamble than their ethnic major- ity peers (Volberg et al. 2010). However, as was observed amongst Pacific young people in the present study, non-majority ethnic youth that do gamble appear to be at greater risk of problem gambling (Delfabbro et al. 2005; Volberg et al. 2010). Previous research has also found that issues with both family and school bonding, or connectedness, are associated with problem gambling (Dickson et al. 2008; Lussier et al. 2007). While this research identified connectedness to friends, family and school as fulfilling protective functions (model one), it is interesting to note that school connectedness was the only domain to maintain this relationship in the presence of other (protective and risk) items (model two). This has important implications for schools in their role in enhancing the overall wellbeing of young people in their possible role towards developing appropri- ate policies (e.g. policies focused on the availability of gambling and reducing the harm Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 10 of 12 caused by problem gambling amongst adolescents), and their input with targeted inter- ventions aimed at preventing gambling problems. This research has identified two socio-ecological domains that are important in rela - tion to youth gambling. Firstly, the gambling behaviour of students’ friends and parents were shown to influence students’ gambling. While social learning and/or social attach - ment theories may assist with the interpretation of this, in-depth qualitative research to fully explore the underlying mechanisms is warranted. Health promotion efforts aimed at education of parents regarding their own acceptance and modelling of gam- bling behaviour, and the subsequent effects on youth may also be useful. Secondly, this research has highlighted the potency of gambling via modes of gambling typically availa- ble at casinos (i.e. EGMs or tables) or the Internet for youth, even when casino gambling for participants in this study was illegal (i.e. casino gambling is only legal in New Zea- land for those aged 20 years or older). This corresponds to research that has previously identified a number of attributes thought to contribute to the potency of these modes, including: limited potential for social monitoring; high levels of accessibility (i.e. casinos operate 24  h a day, seven days a week); being continuous in nature (i.e. there is a very short time frame between investment and outcome); universal access for internet gam- bling (i.e. remotely accessible via smartphones); and, a lack of enforceable age restric- tions for internet gambling (Abbott et al. 2014; Abbott and Volberg 2000; Adams et al. 2004; Floros et  al. 2013; Health Sponsorship Council 2012; Orford 2011). While it will continue to be difficult to enforce age restrictions for gambling via the Internet, there is a clear need for casinos to be extra vigilant about children and adolescents gaining access to their premises. Future research could do more to explore the role of protective factors. For example, it would be valuable to analyse the factors and/or conditions associated with maintaining optimal levels of wellness in the young people that have not gambled, and then poten- tially explore the best ways in which these factors and/or conditions could be enhanced amongst those identified as engaging in problem gambling. More could also be studied in relation to the role of school connectedness, and in particular what it is about a school milieu that protects young people against problem gambling. Strengths and limitations The data presented in this paper are derived from a large nationally representative sample of secondary school students in New Zealand. Questions on gambling were embedded in a holistic survey on overall adolescent health and wellbeing and, as such, gambling can be viewed in a broad socio-ecological context. A limitation of this research is that the sample only included secondary school students. Young people who were dis- engaged from the education system (i.e. have dropped out of school or were truant) were not surveyed, and this may have implications for the generalisability of the findings, par - ticularly as ‘disengaged’ students are likely to be ‘high-risk’ with regard to problematic gambling behaviour (Messerlian and Derevensky 2005). The survey was carried out in 2007, and since this time certain modes of gambling, in particular Internet gambling, have become more widespread. Finally, the Youth’07 survey is a cross-sectional survey which relies on students self-report. While it provides robust correlational data, causal- ity and directionality of relationships cannot be determined. Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 11 of 12 Conclusions Building upon earlier work, such as Blaszczynski and Nower’s Pathway Model, this research used item response theory to identify a number of ‘red flags’ of unhealthy gam - bling behaviour in youth. Socio-ecological factors were also highlighted; schools, fami- lies and the gambling industries are important in fostering safe engagement by youth with gambling. Authors’ contributions FVR, SJD and MFGL carried out the statistical analyses. All authors were involved in the drafting of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript. Author details Social and Community Health, School of Population Health, Faculty of Medical and Health Sciences, University of Auck- land, Private Bag 92019, Auckland 1142, New Zealand. Department of Health and Social Care, The Open University, Mil- 3 4 ton Keynes, UK. Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand. Department of Paediatrics: Child and Youth Health, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand. School of Pharmacy, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand. Competing interests The authors declare that they have no competing interests. Funding Youth’07 was funded by the Health Research Council of New Zealand, the Department of Labour, Families Commis- sion, Accident Corporation of New Zealand, Sport and Recreation New Zealand, The Alcohol Advisory Council of New Zealand, Ministries of Youth Development, Justice and Health. The report on which this study is based was funded by the New Zealand Ministry of Health. Informed consent All procedures were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national) and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000. Informed consent was obtained from all students for being included in the study. Received: 1 March 2016 Accepted: 25 July 2016 References Abbott, M., Bellringer, M., Garrett, N., & Mundy-McPherson, S. (2014). New Zealand 2012 national gambling study: Gambling harm and problem gambling. Report Number Two. Auckland: Gambling and Addictions Research Centre, Auckland University of Technology. Abbott, M., & Volberg, R. A. (2000). Taking the pulse on gambling and problem gambling in New Zealand: A report on phase one of the 1999 national prevalence survey (Report number three of the New Zealand Gaming Survey). Wellington: The Department of Internal Affairs. Adams, P., Rossen, F., Perese, L., Townsend, S., Brown, R., & Garland, J. (2004). Gambling impact assessment for seven Auck- land territorial authorities. Part one: introduction and overview. Auckland: Centre for Gambling Studies, University of Auckland. Adolescent Health Research Group. (2008). Youth’07: The health and wellbeing of secondary school students in New Zealand. Technical report. Auckland: The University of Auckland. Bech, P., Olsen, L. R., Kjoller, M., & Rasmussen, N. K. (2003). Measuring well-being rather than the abscence of distress symptoms: A comparison of the SF-36 Mental Health subscale and the WHO-Five Well-Being Scale. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 12(2), 85–91. Blaszczynski, A., & Nower, L. (2002). A pathways model of problem and pathological gambling. Addiction, 97(5), 487–499. Blinn-Pike, L., Worthy, S. L., & Jonkman, J. N. (2010). Adolescent gambling: A review of an emerging field of research. Journal of Adolescent Health, 47(3), 223–236. Darling, H., Reeder, A. I., McGee, R., & Williams, S. (2006). Brief report: Disposable income, and spending on fast food, alco- hol, cigarettes, and gambling by New Zealand secondary school students. Journal of Adolescence, 29, 834–843. Delfabbro, P., Lahn, J., & Grabosky, P. (2005). Further evidence concerning the prevalence of adolescent gambling and problem gambling in Australia: A study of the ACT. International Gambling Studies, 5(2), 209–228. Derevensky, J., & Gupta, R. (2000). Youth gambling: A clinical and research perspective. Journal of Gambling Issues, 2, 1–11. Derevensky, J. L., Gupta, R., & Winters, K. (2003). Prevalence rates of youth gambling problems: Are the current rates inflated? Journal of Gambling Studies, 19(4), 405–425. Dickson, L., Derevensky, J. L., & Gupta, R. (2008). Youth gambling problems: Examining risk and protective factors. Interna- tional Gambling Studies, 8(1), 25–47. Floros, G., Siomos, K., Fisoun, V., & Geroukalis, D. (2013). Adolescent online gambling: The impact of parental practices and correlates with online activities. Journal of Gambling Studies, 29(1), 131–150. Gupta, R., & Derevensky, J. (1998a). Adolescent gambling behavior: A prevalence study and examination of the correlates associated with problem gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 14(4), 319–345. Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 12 of 12 Gupta, R., & Derevensky, J. (1998b). An empirical examination of Jacobs’ general theory of addictions: Do adolescent gamblers fit the theory? Journal of Gambling Studies, 14(1), 17–49. Gupta, R., & Derevensky, J. (2000). Adolescents with gambling problems: From research to treatment. Journal of Gambling Studies, 16(2/3), 315–342. Gupta, R., Nower, L., Derevensky, J. L., Blaszczynski, A., Faregh, N., & Temcheff, C. (2013). Problem gambling in adolescents: An examination of the pathway model. Journal of Gambling Studies, 29, 575–588. Health Sponsorship Council. (2012). New Zealanders’ knowledge, views and experience of gambling and gambling harm: Results from the 2010 health and lifestyles survey. Wellington, New Zealand: Health Sponsorship Council. Retrieved from http://www.hsc.org.nz/researchpublications.html. Huang, J.-H., & Boyer, R. (2007). Epidemiology of youth gambling problems in Canada: A national prevalence study. Cana- dian Journal of Psychiatry - Revue Canadienne de Psychiatrie, 52(10), 657–665. Jacobs, D. F. (2000). Juvenile gambling in North America: An analysis of long term trends and future prospects. Journal of Gambling Studies, 16(2–3), 119–152. Lussier, I., Derevensky, J. L., Gupta, R., Bergevin, T., & Ellenbogen, S. (2007). Youth gambling behaviors: An examination of the role of resilience. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 21(2), 165–173. Messerlian, C., & Derevensky, J. (2005). Youth gambling: A public health perspective. Journal of Gambling Issues,. doi:10.4309/jgi.2005.14.9. Muthen, L., & Muthen, B. (1998–2007). Mplus user’s guide (5th edn.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen. Norris, M. L. (2007). HEADSS up: Adolescents and the internet. Paediatrics and Child Health, 12(3), 211–216. Orford, J. (2011). An unsafe bet? The dangerous rise of gambling and the debate we should be having. Chichester: Wiley Blackwell. Raisamo, S., Halme, J., Murto, A., & Lintonen, T. (2013). Gambling-related harms among adolescents: A population-based study. Journal of Gambling Studies, 29, 151–159. Salmond, C., Crampton, P., Sutton, F., & Atkinson, J. (2006). NZDep2006 census area unit data. Retrieved Sept 19, 2006, from http://www.wnmeds.ac.nz/academic/dph/research/socialindicators.html. Shead, N. W., Derevensky, J. L., & Gupta, R. (2010). Risk and protective factors associated with youth problem gambling. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine & Health, 22(1), 39–58. Splevins, K., Mireskandari, S., Clayton, K., & Blaszczynski, A. (2010). Prevalence of adolescent problem gambling, related harms and help-seeking behaviours among an Australian population. Journal of Gambling Studies, 26(2), 189–204. Ste-Marie, C., Derevensky, J., & Gupta, R. (2002). Anxiety and social stress related to adolescent gambling behavior. Inter- national Gambling Studies, 2, 123–141. Turchi, R. M., & Derevensky, J. L. (2006). Youth gambling: Not a safe bet. Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 18(4), 454–458. Volberg, R., Gupta, R., Griffiths, M., Ólason, D., & Delfabbro, P. (2010). An international perspective on youth gambling prevalence studies. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine & Health, 22(1), 3–38. http://www.deepdyve.com/assets/images/DeepDyve-Logo-lg.png Asian Journal of Gambling Issues and Public Health Springer Journals

Adolescent gambling behaviour, a single latent construct and indicators of risk: findings from a national survey of New Zealand high school students

Loading next page...
 
/lp/springer-journals/adolescent-gambling-behaviour-a-single-latent-construct-and-indicators-uIBiaeqNYe
Publisher
Springer Journals
Copyright
Copyright © 2016 by The Author(s)
Subject
Social Sciences; Social Sciences, general; Quality of Life Research; Sociology, general; Public Health; Community and Environmental Psychology
eISSN
2195-3007
DOI
10.1186/s40405-016-0017-9
Publisher site
See Article on Publisher Site

Abstract

f.rossen@auckland.ac.nz Social and Community This study explores underlying latent construct/s of gambling behaviour, and identifies Health, School of Population indicators of “unhealthy gambling”. Data were collected from Youth’07 a nationally rep- Health, Faculty of Medical and Health Sciences, resentative sample of New Zealand secondary school students (N = 9107). Exploratory University of Auckland, factor analyses, item-response theory analyses, multiple indicators-multiple causes, and Private Bag 92019, differential item functioning analyses were used to assess dimensionality of gambling Auckland 1142, New Zealand Full list of author information behaviour, underlying factors, and indicators of unhealthy gambling. A single underly- is available at the end of the ing continuum of gambling behaviour was identified. Gambling frequency and ‘gam- article bling because I can’t stop’ were most strongly associated with unhealthy gambling. Gambling to ‘feel better about myself ’ and to ‘forget about things’ provided the most precise discriminants of unhealthy gambling. Multivariable analyses found that school connectedness was associated with lower levels of unhealthy gambling. Background Gambling has become a widely available activity in modern societies (Turchi and Der- evensky 2006). Evidence suggests that it has become a popular past-time, not only for adults, but also for children and adolescents (Derevensky and Gupta 2000; Gupta and Derevensky 1998a; Jacobs 2000; Splevins et  al. 2010; Turchi and Derevensky 2006). Whilst for many youth involvement in gambling does not result in problematic behav- iour, other adolescents go on to experience serious issues as a result of their gambling. While the documented rates of youth involvement in gambling vary widely (perhaps due to methodological discrepancies), a recent review of studies of young people’s gambling in North America, Europe and Oceania reported that 20–90  % of young people had gambled in the past year (Volberg et al. 2010) and 0.8–13.0 % were involved in problem/ pathological gambling (Volberg et al. 2010). Certain groups of young people are at greater risk of unhealthy gambling. For instance, male youth are more likely than their female counterparts to engage in gambling (Blinn- Pike et  al. 2010; Darling et  al. 2006; Delfabbro et  al. 2005; Huang and Boyer 2007; Raisamo et  al. 2013; Turchi and Derevensky 2006), gamble more frequently (Delfabbro et  al. 2005; Gupta and Derevensky 1998a, b) and do so on a wider range of activities © 2016 The Author(s). This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 2 of 12 (Delfabbro et  al. 2005). The prevalence of problem gambling amongst youth from dif - ferent ethnic groups has not been widely investigated (Blinn-Pike et  al. 2010). Despite this lack of evidence, a few studies have indicated that indigenous youth and those from non-majority ethnic groups may be less likely to gamble than their peers. However those non-majority ethnic youth that do gamble are at greater risk of experiencing problem gambling (Delfabbro et al. 2005; Volberg et al. 2010). The literature has highlighted that adolescent problem gamblers have an array of co- existing problems (Gupta and Derevensky 2000; Shead et al. 2010; Volberg et al. 2010), with researchers theorising that gambling is often undertaken as an attempt to manage or resolve other underlying issues (Gupta and Derevensky 2000; Ste-Marie et al. 2002). Furthermore, risk factors associated with problem gambling, such as mental health issues (e.g. depression and anxiety) may also be observed in other potentially harmful behaviours in adolescence, for instance substance misuse (Derevensky et al. 2003; Shead et  al. 2010). While there has been some focus on the identification of risk factors for youth problem gambling, little is known about the protective factors that insulate young people against unhealthy gambling (Dickson et al. 2008), or the extent of their positive influence over risk factors. There is a dearth of information on the ‘warning signs’ for problem gambling, mean - ing that preventive measures can be hampered. Policy makers and those working clini- cally in the problem gambling field would benefit from an exploration of indicators of when gambling behaviour is becoming unhealthy, and a non-dichotomised examination of youth gambling would allow for a comprehensive analysis of factors associated with unhealthy gambling. This study presents findings from research which explored youth participation in gambling and the impact of problem gambling, and identifiable risk and resiliency factors. To the best of the authors’ knowledge this is the first systematic assess - ment of risk and protective factors of youth gambling based on data from a nationally representative sample of young people. The objectives of this study were to: (1) explore the existence of an underlying latent construct/s of gambling behaviour—i.e. to create an underlying latent continuum of gambling behaviours from ‘less unhealthy’ to ‘more unhealthy’; (2) identify ‘red flags’, or indicators of unhealthy gambling behaviours; and, (3) explore and identify associations between gambling behaviour (as measured by the latent construct), demographic variables, and variables that were hypothesised to fulfil risk or protective factors. Methods Sample Data for this study were collected as part of Youth’07, a nationally representative sur- vey of the health and wellbeing of New Zealand secondary school students conducted in 2007. A brief overview of the Youth’07 survey design and methodology is included below, while a comprehensive description has been provided elsewhere (Adolescent Health Research Group 2008). A total of 9107 randomly selected secondary schools students (equating to 3.4  % of the total 2007 New Zealand secondary school roll) aged between 12 and 19 years old completed the survey. Final response rates were 84 % for schools and 74 % for students. The comprehensive 622-question survey was administered via inter - net tablets (hand held computers). Screening questions were used to ensure that only Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 3 of 12 those who had experienced a particular issue were asked more detailed questions (e.g. those who had not smoked cigarettes were not asked further questions about smoking). Hence, students generally answered considerably fewer questions than the survey total number of questions. Measures Involvement in gambling was assessed through a series of questions. The first question determined past year gambling: (“In the last year have you ever gambled or bet money on things like Lotto, Instant Kiwi, Pokies etc., or bet money with friends?”). Students who answered affirmatively were then asked five questions to determine the extent of their involvement in gambling behaviour over the past year, in particular: • Gambling activities “In the last year have you gambled or bet money on any of these?” Students could select as many of the following options as appropriate—Instant Kiwi (scratchies); Lotto (including Strike and Powerball); bingo or housie; pub or club pok- ies (i.e. electronic gambling machines/EGMs); casino pokies or tables (e.g. roulette); TAB betting; internet (e.g. internet casinos); bets with friends; 0900 phone games; cards or coins; other; and/or, none of these. • Frequency of gambling “During the last four weeks, about how often did you gam- ble?” Affirmative responses to this item were aggregated to ‘several times a week/ most days a week,’ and the broader category ‘once a week or less’. • Money spent on gambling “How much money would you usually spend each week on bets or gambling?” Responses to this item were collapsed into two categories—‘less than NZ$20’ (less than approximately US$13) and ‘NZ$20 or more’. • Time spent gambling “How much time would you usually spend each day on bets or gambling?” Responses were aggregated to ‘less than 1 h a day’ and ‘1 or more hours per day’. • Reasons for gambling “Why do you gamble or bet money?” Students were able to select as many reasons as appropriate from the following list: “to have fun”; “to win money”; “because I am bored”; “to relax”; “to feel better about myself ”; “to forget about things”; “because my friends do”; “because my family does”; “for a challenge”; “because I can’t stop”; “because I am short of money”; “to get a buzz”; and, “none of these”. Variables hypothesised to fulfil risk or protective factors Variables selected for inclusion in this set of analyses were identified a priori by review - ing the literature and based on consultation with the study’s advisory group, which con- sisted of researchers and clinicians with expertise in the prevention and treatment of problem gambling. • Wellbeing was assessed by the WHO-Five Wellbeing Index (Bech et  al. 2003). This Index is made up of five items that use a six-point Likert scale, from 0 (at no time) to 6 (all of the time). Three underlying constructs are measured: positive mood, vital - ity, and general interests. Response scores were summed to provide an overall score, with higher scores indicating greater wellbeing. Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 4 of 12 • Depression was measured by the Reynolds Adolescent Depression Scale-Short Form (RADS-SF) (Norris 2007). This scale consists of ten items, each of which has four response options: “almost never” “hardly ever”, “sometimes”, and “most of the time” (significant depressive symptoms  ≥ 28, total score = 40). The overall score provides an indication of affective status, with higher scores being an indicator of greater depressive symptomatology. • Data on weekly alcohol use were gathered by asking those students who had drank alcohol at least once in their lifetime “During the past 4 weeks, about how often did you drink alcohol?”; responses to this item were aggregated to ‘weekly or more often’ and ‘less than weekly’. • Scales were developed to provide measures of social connectedness in three domains: family, friends, and school. The scale for family connectedness included nine items that focused on: whether students had fun with their family; if they got along with their family; and, their relationship with their family, in particular their mother and/ or father (or people that functioned as parents). The scale for connectedness to friends incorporated six items which covered various aspects of friendships, such as having caring friends and fun with friends. The school connectedness scale included eight items that enquired about students’ connectedness to their school. All three scales demonstrated adequate to good internal consistency, with Cronbach’s alphas of 0.85 (family), 0.78 (friends) and 0.60 (school). • Weekly income there was no specific question about whether or not students received a weekly allowance, hence income was determined by asking students “How much money do you usually earn each week?” • Age, sex, and ethnicity were self-reported. • Neighbourhood socio-economic deprivation was measured using the New Zealand Deprivation Index (NZDep2006) (Salmond et al. 2006). NZDep2006 was also used to identify whether students resided in urban or rural settings. Analysis The analyses for this study were completed in 2015 and includes only those students who had gambled in the past year from the total Youth’07 sample of 9107 students (26.7  %; n  =  2234). These students were significantly (p  ≤  0.05) more likely to be male (31.3  % male and 21.5 % female), older (24.9 % ‘13 or less’, 23.7 % ‘14 years old’, 26.2 % ‘15 years old’, 28.5 % ‘16 years old’, 31.9 % ‘17 or older’), and from neighbourhoods with lower lev- els of socio-economic deprivation (28.2 % low, 27.5 % medium, 23.3 % high). No differ - ences were observed for past-year gambling status according to ethnicity or rural/urban category. Exploratory factor analysis was used to assess dimensionality of gambling behaviour and the number of underlying factors. The relationship between participants’ responses to each item and their level on the underlying latent gambling continuum/s were exam- ined using two-parameter logistic item-response theory (IRT) models. Item characteris- tic curves (ICC) illustrate these relationships, which were characterised by item severity and discrimination parameters. Item severity provides an indication of the position of the item characteristic curve in relation to the latent continuum: higher severity val- ues indicate that an item is associated with a higher severity of gambling behaviour (i.e. Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 5 of 12 unhealthy gambling behaviour). Discrimination parameters provide an indication of the accuracy or precision of each item in distinguishing between those participants with lev- els of the latent continuum above, and those participants with levels below, the item’s severity. A low discrimination estimate indicates that an item is unrelated to the under- lying construct or that the item is poorly defined. Multiple indicators-multiple causes (MIMIC) analyses were used to examine the rela- tionship between demographic variables and the underlying continuum of problem gambling behaviours and predictors of unhealthy gambling behaviour. Differential Item Functioning (DIF) by demographic variables (i.e. age, sex and ethnicity) was used to examine differences in how demographic groups respond to individual questions, inde - pendent of the underlying problem gambling continuum. The presence of DIF would indicate measurement non-equivalence or item response bias across groups. Analysis of risk and protective factors progressed in two models. The first model examined independent associations between the latent gambling continuum and risk and protective factors, whilst accounting for student-level demographics. In model two, the association of all significant risk and protective factors and student-level demo - graphics were examined simultaneously in relation to the latent gambling continuum. Multivariable, factor, IRT, and MIMIC analyses were undertaken using Mplus (Muthen and Muthen 1998–2007). All analyses accounted for the clustering of students within the same school and the unequal probability of selection. Human participant protection Principals provided consent on behalf of their schools and students that were selected and their parents were provided with information sheets about the study. Students provided their consent on the day of participating. Ethical approval for the study was granted by the University of Auckland Human Subject Ethics Committee. Results Underlying constructs of unhealthy gambling behaviour Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was used to assess the dimensionality of the gambling behaviour items relating to reasons for gambling, frequency of gambling, and time and money spent gambling. The scree plot of the eigenvalues (5.301, 0.838, 0.294) and the ratio of the first to the second eigenvalue (5.301/0.838  = 6.326) suggested the presence of a single, dominant factor underlying these gambling items. This one-factor model demonstrated excellent fit indices (RMSEA  = 0.047, CFI = 0.982, TLI = 0.973), and pro- vided evidence of undimensionality of the variables, meaning that it was suitable for IRT modelling (see Table  1 for details on item factor loadings, discrimination and severity parameters). This factor was conceptualised as an underlying continuum of ‘unhealthy’ gambling behaviour and centres on motivations/reasons for gambling and levels of expenditure (in terms of time, frequency, and money) on gambling. Identifying the ‘red flags’ of unhealthy gambling behaviour Figure  1 illustrates the item characteristic curves for the gambling items. All of the gambling items exhibited good to high discriminative power (range 1.284–5.044) in Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 6 of 12 Table 1 Two-parameter item-response theory model of gambling behaviour Prevalence Factor loadings Item discrimination Item severity (%) Estimate Standard error Estimate Standard error I gamble to relax 6.0 0.848 1.603 0.133 1.841 0.068 I gamble to feel 2.9 0.981 5.044 1.913 1.928 0.060 better about myself I gamble to forget 3.7 0.914 2.246 0.256 1.954 0.073 about things I gamble because 1.6 0.885 1.902 0.295 2.431 0.116 I can’t stop Gamble ‘several 2.6 0.806 1.361 0.206 2.412 0.159 times a week’ or ‘most days’ Spend $20 or 5.0 0.789 1.284 0.123 2.101 0.100 more per week on gambling Spend 1 or more 3.1 0.866 1.730 0.240 2.189 0.101 hours per day gambling Fig. 1 Item response curve distinguishing behaviour along the gambling continuum, from less unhealthy to more unhealthy behaviour. Gambling “to feel better about myself ” was the item with the high- est discrimination (5.044, note the steepest slope in Fig.  1) and was most able to accu- rately discriminate students along the gambling continuum. Gambling “to forget about things”, “because I can’t stop”, and “to relax” also had high item discriminations (2.246, 1.902, and 1.603 respectively). The amount of money spent on gambling had the lowest item discrimination (1.284; note the least steep slope), meaning that it was least able to accurately discriminate students along the continuum of gambling behaviour. As gam- bling “to relax”, “to feel better about myself ”, and “to forget about things” had the lowest item severities, these items may therefore act as early indicators that gambling behaviour is more unhealthy. Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 7 of 12 Unhealthy gambling, depression and wellbeing Measures of depression and wellbeing were used to determine construct validation. The underlying gambling behaviour continuum was significantly correlated with standard - ised measures of depression (RADS-SF) and wellbeing (WHO-Five Wellbeing Index). ‘More unhealthy’ gambling behaviour was positively associated with depression and negatively associated with wellbeing, in that students with ‘more unhealthy’ gambling behaviour had significantly higher depression scores (r  =  0.162, p  <  0.001) and signifi - cantly lower wellbeing scores (r = −0.113, p < 0.001) than students who were gambling at ‘less unhealthy’ levels. Unhealthy gambling behaviour and different demographic groups MIMIC modelling was carried out to determine if there were significant direct effects between demographic variables (i.e. age, sex, and ethnicity) and the gambling items, and whether any of the demographic subgroups were at increased or decreased risk of unhealthy gambling behaviours  (see Fig.  2). Differential Item Functioning (DIF) dem - onstrated item equivalence for seven gambling items (gambling “to relax”, gambling “to feel better”, gambling “to forget”, gambling “because can’t stop”, frequency of gam- bling, money spent gambling, and time spent gambling) across the age, sex and ethnic groups of students. The MIMIC model provided a good fit for the data (RMSEA  = 0.030, CFI = 0.979, TLI = 0.973) and ‘unhealthy gambling’ behaviour was shown to vary across each of the investigated demographic variables. Higher levels of ‘unhealthy gambling’ behaviour were associated with being younger, male, and being an ethnic minority. Risk and protective factors for gambling behaviour Analyses for Model One resulted in a set of variables that were significantly associated with scores on the gambling continuum (see Table  2 for estimates and standard errors) while adjusting for age, sex, ethnicity and neighbourhood socio-economic deprivation. Ten variables were associated with increased risk/higher scores on the latent gambling continuum (i.e. ‘more unhealthy’ gambling behaviour). Three domains were negatively associated with scores on the gambling continuum, i.e. they were protective with regard to unhealthy gambling behaviour: connectedness to family, connectedness to friends, and connectedness to school. As levels of connectedness in each of these domains increased, the mean score on the gambling continuum decreased. Model Two entailed a multivariate analysis between the latent gambling continuum, demographic variables, and all of the variables identified in Model One as independently fulfilling risk/protective functions. The analysis in Model Two aimed to determine if those variables identified as fulfilling risk/protective functions (as identified in Model One) would continue to maintain these functions in the presence of each other. The only variable to maintain its protective status was school connectedness (p  =  0.018). Vari- ables that continued to be significantly associated with increased risk included being of a Pacific ethnicity (p  = 0.009), earning more than NZ$100 per week (p = 0.031), being male (p  =  0.002), gambling because friends gamble (p  <  0.001), and gambling because family gambles (p < 0.001). Gambling at a casino (on EGMs or tables) and gambling over the Internet were significantly associated with an increased risk of ‘unhealthy gambling’ Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 8 of 12 Gamble to relax YOUNGER STUDENTS Gamble to feel better about -0.093* (-0.067) myself MALE vs. FEMALE Gamble to forget about things 0.202* (0.417) UNHEALTHY MAORI vs. EUROPEAN GAMBLING Gamble because I can’t stop 0.182* (0.478) BEHAVIOUR Gamble ‘several times a PACIFIC vs. EUROPEAN week’ or ‘most days’ 0.237* (0.870) ASIAN vs. EUROPEAN Spend $20 or more per week 0.109* (0.336) on gambling Spend 1 or more hours per Regression coefficient (standard error); *P value <0.05 day gambling Fig. 2 MIMIC model of unhealthy gambling behaviour. Relates to students who have gambled in the past 12 months (N = 2234) behaviour (p = 0.006 and p = 0.001 respectively). Table 2 provides estimates and stand- ard errors for each of these items. Discussion Based on data from a nationally representative sample of secondary school students, the current study has identified a single construct of unhealthy gambling behaviour and a number of ‘red flags’, or indicators, of when gambling behaviour is at or is close to unhealthy levels. For instance, “gambling to relax”, “gambling to feel better about myself ”, and “gambling to forget about things” are probable early indicators that gambling behav- iour may be at unhealthy levels. It may be useful to incorporate these ‘red flags’ in health promotion and treatment initiatives so that young people and those responsible for their health and wellbeing (e.g. teachers, parents, and clinicians) have an awareness of the warning signs linked to unhealthy gambling behaviour. The statistical model and ‘red flags’ that were identified in this research resemble a number of aspects of Blaszczynski and Nower’s (2002) second pathway—emotionally vulnerable problem gamblers, from their theoretical Pathway Model of problem and pathological gambling conducted with adults. They propose that emotionally vulner - able gamblers are motivated to participate in gambling “by a desire to modulate affective states and/or meet specific psychological needs” (p. 492). Recent research in adolescent problem gambling has further supported Blaszczynski and Nower’s Pathway Model (Gupta et  al. 2013), and as in previous research the three ‘red flags’ of unhealthy gam - bling identified in the present study all appear to centre on reducing states of arousal. Moreover, in our study unhealthy levels of gambling were associated with depression, poorer wellbeing, and weekly use of alcohol. Blaszczynski and Nower (2002) also argue that the underlying psychological dysfunction present in ‘pathway two’ gamblers makes Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 9 of 12 Table 2 Multi-model correlations between  risk and  protective factors for  unhealthy gam- bling behaviour Item/variable associated with increased Model 1 Model 2 risk of unhealthy gambling Estimate (Standard error)* Estimate (Standard error)* Demographics Age (younger vs. older) – −0.092 (0.065) Sex (male vs. female) – 0.580 (0.187)* Māori (vs. European) – 0.227 (0.177) Pacific (vs. European) – 0.736 (0.282)* Asian (vs. European) – 0.535 (0.288) Living in neighbourhood with higher levels of dep- – 0.044 (0.032) rivation (vs. lower levels of deprivation) Risk factors Usually earns $100 or more per week 0.387 (0.111)* 0.322 (0.150)* Gamble because My friends gamble 1.436 (0.122)* 1.130 (0.231)* My family gamble 1.53 (0.13)* 1.161 (0.237)* Have gambled on Bingo 1.102 (0.133)* 0.402 (0.296) Pub/club EGMs 0.972 (0.118)* 0.199 (0.269) Casino (EGMs or tables) 1.514 (0.158)* 0.939 (0.339)* Internet 1.32 (0.114)* 0.864 (0.256)* Phone 1.637 (0.18)* 0.548 (0.474) Satisfied depression criteria 0.496 (0.128)* 0.261 (0.266) Drinks alcohol on a weekly basis 0.649 (0.078)* 0.246 (0.175) Protective factors Connectedness to family −0.245 (0.052)* 0.068 (0.103) Connectedness to friends −0.33 (0.038)* −0.053 (0.103) Connectedness to school −0.412 (0.077)* −0.326 (0.137)* * Item/variable significant at p ≤ 0.01 Adjusted for age, sex, ethnicity and neighbourhood socio-economic deprivation (NZDep2006) them more resistant to treatment and that underlying vulnerabilities, such as substance misuse, must be addressed alongside gambling issues. As in the current study, prior work has indicated that those from non-majority ethnic groups (e.g. Native American and African American youth in the USA and indigenous young people in Australia) may not be any more likely to gamble than their ethnic major- ity peers (Volberg et al. 2010). However, as was observed amongst Pacific young people in the present study, non-majority ethnic youth that do gamble appear to be at greater risk of problem gambling (Delfabbro et al. 2005; Volberg et al. 2010). Previous research has also found that issues with both family and school bonding, or connectedness, are associated with problem gambling (Dickson et al. 2008; Lussier et al. 2007). While this research identified connectedness to friends, family and school as fulfilling protective functions (model one), it is interesting to note that school connectedness was the only domain to maintain this relationship in the presence of other (protective and risk) items (model two). This has important implications for schools in their role in enhancing the overall wellbeing of young people in their possible role towards developing appropri- ate policies (e.g. policies focused on the availability of gambling and reducing the harm Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 10 of 12 caused by problem gambling amongst adolescents), and their input with targeted inter- ventions aimed at preventing gambling problems. This research has identified two socio-ecological domains that are important in rela - tion to youth gambling. Firstly, the gambling behaviour of students’ friends and parents were shown to influence students’ gambling. While social learning and/or social attach - ment theories may assist with the interpretation of this, in-depth qualitative research to fully explore the underlying mechanisms is warranted. Health promotion efforts aimed at education of parents regarding their own acceptance and modelling of gam- bling behaviour, and the subsequent effects on youth may also be useful. Secondly, this research has highlighted the potency of gambling via modes of gambling typically availa- ble at casinos (i.e. EGMs or tables) or the Internet for youth, even when casino gambling for participants in this study was illegal (i.e. casino gambling is only legal in New Zea- land for those aged 20 years or older). This corresponds to research that has previously identified a number of attributes thought to contribute to the potency of these modes, including: limited potential for social monitoring; high levels of accessibility (i.e. casinos operate 24  h a day, seven days a week); being continuous in nature (i.e. there is a very short time frame between investment and outcome); universal access for internet gam- bling (i.e. remotely accessible via smartphones); and, a lack of enforceable age restric- tions for internet gambling (Abbott et al. 2014; Abbott and Volberg 2000; Adams et al. 2004; Floros et  al. 2013; Health Sponsorship Council 2012; Orford 2011). While it will continue to be difficult to enforce age restrictions for gambling via the Internet, there is a clear need for casinos to be extra vigilant about children and adolescents gaining access to their premises. Future research could do more to explore the role of protective factors. For example, it would be valuable to analyse the factors and/or conditions associated with maintaining optimal levels of wellness in the young people that have not gambled, and then poten- tially explore the best ways in which these factors and/or conditions could be enhanced amongst those identified as engaging in problem gambling. More could also be studied in relation to the role of school connectedness, and in particular what it is about a school milieu that protects young people against problem gambling. Strengths and limitations The data presented in this paper are derived from a large nationally representative sample of secondary school students in New Zealand. Questions on gambling were embedded in a holistic survey on overall adolescent health and wellbeing and, as such, gambling can be viewed in a broad socio-ecological context. A limitation of this research is that the sample only included secondary school students. Young people who were dis- engaged from the education system (i.e. have dropped out of school or were truant) were not surveyed, and this may have implications for the generalisability of the findings, par - ticularly as ‘disengaged’ students are likely to be ‘high-risk’ with regard to problematic gambling behaviour (Messerlian and Derevensky 2005). The survey was carried out in 2007, and since this time certain modes of gambling, in particular Internet gambling, have become more widespread. Finally, the Youth’07 survey is a cross-sectional survey which relies on students self-report. While it provides robust correlational data, causal- ity and directionality of relationships cannot be determined. Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 11 of 12 Conclusions Building upon earlier work, such as Blaszczynski and Nower’s Pathway Model, this research used item response theory to identify a number of ‘red flags’ of unhealthy gam - bling behaviour in youth. Socio-ecological factors were also highlighted; schools, fami- lies and the gambling industries are important in fostering safe engagement by youth with gambling. Authors’ contributions FVR, SJD and MFGL carried out the statistical analyses. All authors were involved in the drafting of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript. Author details Social and Community Health, School of Population Health, Faculty of Medical and Health Sciences, University of Auck- land, Private Bag 92019, Auckland 1142, New Zealand. Department of Health and Social Care, The Open University, Mil- 3 4 ton Keynes, UK. Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand. Department of Paediatrics: Child and Youth Health, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand. School of Pharmacy, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand. Competing interests The authors declare that they have no competing interests. Funding Youth’07 was funded by the Health Research Council of New Zealand, the Department of Labour, Families Commis- sion, Accident Corporation of New Zealand, Sport and Recreation New Zealand, The Alcohol Advisory Council of New Zealand, Ministries of Youth Development, Justice and Health. The report on which this study is based was funded by the New Zealand Ministry of Health. Informed consent All procedures were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national) and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000. Informed consent was obtained from all students for being included in the study. Received: 1 March 2016 Accepted: 25 July 2016 References Abbott, M., Bellringer, M., Garrett, N., & Mundy-McPherson, S. (2014). New Zealand 2012 national gambling study: Gambling harm and problem gambling. Report Number Two. Auckland: Gambling and Addictions Research Centre, Auckland University of Technology. Abbott, M., & Volberg, R. A. (2000). Taking the pulse on gambling and problem gambling in New Zealand: A report on phase one of the 1999 national prevalence survey (Report number three of the New Zealand Gaming Survey). Wellington: The Department of Internal Affairs. Adams, P., Rossen, F., Perese, L., Townsend, S., Brown, R., & Garland, J. (2004). Gambling impact assessment for seven Auck- land territorial authorities. Part one: introduction and overview. Auckland: Centre for Gambling Studies, University of Auckland. Adolescent Health Research Group. (2008). Youth’07: The health and wellbeing of secondary school students in New Zealand. Technical report. Auckland: The University of Auckland. Bech, P., Olsen, L. R., Kjoller, M., & Rasmussen, N. K. (2003). Measuring well-being rather than the abscence of distress symptoms: A comparison of the SF-36 Mental Health subscale and the WHO-Five Well-Being Scale. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 12(2), 85–91. Blaszczynski, A., & Nower, L. (2002). A pathways model of problem and pathological gambling. Addiction, 97(5), 487–499. Blinn-Pike, L., Worthy, S. L., & Jonkman, J. N. (2010). Adolescent gambling: A review of an emerging field of research. Journal of Adolescent Health, 47(3), 223–236. Darling, H., Reeder, A. I., McGee, R., & Williams, S. (2006). Brief report: Disposable income, and spending on fast food, alco- hol, cigarettes, and gambling by New Zealand secondary school students. Journal of Adolescence, 29, 834–843. Delfabbro, P., Lahn, J., & Grabosky, P. (2005). Further evidence concerning the prevalence of adolescent gambling and problem gambling in Australia: A study of the ACT. International Gambling Studies, 5(2), 209–228. Derevensky, J., & Gupta, R. (2000). Youth gambling: A clinical and research perspective. Journal of Gambling Issues, 2, 1–11. Derevensky, J. L., Gupta, R., & Winters, K. (2003). Prevalence rates of youth gambling problems: Are the current rates inflated? Journal of Gambling Studies, 19(4), 405–425. Dickson, L., Derevensky, J. L., & Gupta, R. (2008). Youth gambling problems: Examining risk and protective factors. Interna- tional Gambling Studies, 8(1), 25–47. Floros, G., Siomos, K., Fisoun, V., & Geroukalis, D. (2013). Adolescent online gambling: The impact of parental practices and correlates with online activities. Journal of Gambling Studies, 29(1), 131–150. Gupta, R., & Derevensky, J. (1998a). Adolescent gambling behavior: A prevalence study and examination of the correlates associated with problem gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies, 14(4), 319–345. Rossen et al. Asian J of Gambling Issues and Public Health (2016) 6:7 Page 12 of 12 Gupta, R., & Derevensky, J. (1998b). An empirical examination of Jacobs’ general theory of addictions: Do adolescent gamblers fit the theory? Journal of Gambling Studies, 14(1), 17–49. Gupta, R., & Derevensky, J. (2000). Adolescents with gambling problems: From research to treatment. Journal of Gambling Studies, 16(2/3), 315–342. Gupta, R., Nower, L., Derevensky, J. L., Blaszczynski, A., Faregh, N., & Temcheff, C. (2013). Problem gambling in adolescents: An examination of the pathway model. Journal of Gambling Studies, 29, 575–588. Health Sponsorship Council. (2012). New Zealanders’ knowledge, views and experience of gambling and gambling harm: Results from the 2010 health and lifestyles survey. Wellington, New Zealand: Health Sponsorship Council. Retrieved from http://www.hsc.org.nz/researchpublications.html. Huang, J.-H., & Boyer, R. (2007). Epidemiology of youth gambling problems in Canada: A national prevalence study. Cana- dian Journal of Psychiatry - Revue Canadienne de Psychiatrie, 52(10), 657–665. Jacobs, D. F. (2000). Juvenile gambling in North America: An analysis of long term trends and future prospects. Journal of Gambling Studies, 16(2–3), 119–152. Lussier, I., Derevensky, J. L., Gupta, R., Bergevin, T., & Ellenbogen, S. (2007). Youth gambling behaviors: An examination of the role of resilience. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 21(2), 165–173. Messerlian, C., & Derevensky, J. (2005). Youth gambling: A public health perspective. Journal of Gambling Issues,. doi:10.4309/jgi.2005.14.9. Muthen, L., & Muthen, B. (1998–2007). Mplus user’s guide (5th edn.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthen & Muthen. Norris, M. L. (2007). HEADSS up: Adolescents and the internet. Paediatrics and Child Health, 12(3), 211–216. Orford, J. (2011). An unsafe bet? The dangerous rise of gambling and the debate we should be having. Chichester: Wiley Blackwell. Raisamo, S., Halme, J., Murto, A., & Lintonen, T. (2013). Gambling-related harms among adolescents: A population-based study. Journal of Gambling Studies, 29, 151–159. Salmond, C., Crampton, P., Sutton, F., & Atkinson, J. (2006). NZDep2006 census area unit data. Retrieved Sept 19, 2006, from http://www.wnmeds.ac.nz/academic/dph/research/socialindicators.html. Shead, N. W., Derevensky, J. L., & Gupta, R. (2010). Risk and protective factors associated with youth problem gambling. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine & Health, 22(1), 39–58. Splevins, K., Mireskandari, S., Clayton, K., & Blaszczynski, A. (2010). Prevalence of adolescent problem gambling, related harms and help-seeking behaviours among an Australian population. Journal of Gambling Studies, 26(2), 189–204. Ste-Marie, C., Derevensky, J., & Gupta, R. (2002). Anxiety and social stress related to adolescent gambling behavior. Inter- national Gambling Studies, 2, 123–141. Turchi, R. M., & Derevensky, J. L. (2006). Youth gambling: Not a safe bet. Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 18(4), 454–458. Volberg, R., Gupta, R., Griffiths, M., Ólason, D., & Delfabbro, P. (2010). An international perspective on youth gambling prevalence studies. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine & Health, 22(1), 3–38.

Journal

Asian Journal of Gambling Issues and Public HealthSpringer Journals

Published: Aug 3, 2016

References